Indian Journal of Medical Specialities Trust
 
Original article
 
Effect of pulse pressure on cognitive function
 
Nasreen Akhtar, Shivani Agarwal, Rajesh Gautam, Rashmi Babbar
Department of Physiology, Maulana Azad Medical College, New Delhi, India

Corresponding Author: Dr Nasreen Akhtar, Dept of Physiology, Maulana Azad Medical College, New Delhi-110002. India      Email: drnasreenakhtar@gmail.com

Abstract

Although the effects of blood pressure on cardiovascular events and cognition have been reported widely in literature, the effect of pulse pressure on cognition has received less attention. In this study 65 male subjects with essential hypertension, not on any treatment, with a pulse pressure of more than 60 mmHg (mean pulse pressure-70.76±11.50 mmHg) were evaluated. Mini Mental State Examination and Event related potentials (P300 and N200) were used for assessment of cognitive function. 65 age and sex matched non-hypertensive controls with a pulse pressure less than 60 mmHg (mean pulse pressure-43.87±8.79) were taken as controls. Compared to controls (P300 latency- Fz-310.2 ± 16.71, Cz- 305.3 ± 16.10, Pz- 316.1 ± 14.55. N200 latency- Fz- 193.4 ± 11.78, Cz- 187.3 ± 12.36, Pz- 198.3 ± 15.21), the cases had increased latency of P300 and N200 waveforms (P300 latency- Fz -367.72 ± 14.35, Cz- 361.36 ± 16.77, Pz- 375.45 ± 14.98. N200 latency- Fz-243.21 ± 9.33, Cz- 231.0 ± 12.57, Pz- 248.22 ± 10.51) signifying impaired cognitive functions.

Key words: Pulse pressure, cognition, P300, ERPs, MMSE, hypertension

Introduction
The current guidelines for the management of hypertension rest almost completely on the measurement of systolic and diastolic blood pressures, two specific inflection points of the blood pressure (BP) wave, which are usually considered in isolation [1,2]. However, BP propagates through the arterial tree as a repetitive continuous wave and is more accurately described as consisting of a pulsatile component (pulse pressure) and a steady component (mean pressure) [3]. Pulse pressure is the difference between systolic and diastolic BP and depends on ventricular ejection, arterial stiffness and the timing of wave reflections.

A few population based studies have linked high blood pressure to an increased risk of cardiac and cerebrovascular events [4,5,6]. Vascular risk factors have also been linked to the pathogenesis of cognitive disorders and dementia [7]. However, the potential damage on cognitive function by the different BP components is less clear and has not been examined to the same extent as BP effects on cardiovascular disease and total mortality. Since increased pulse pressure is a clinical indicator of arterial stiffness, it could be postulated that functional changes of the arterial system are involved in the pathogenesis of cognitive decline and dementia [8]. Nonetheless, research so far has failed to establish a link between raised pulse pressure and cognitive decline.

Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) is a brief, objective assessment of cognitive functioning and is a reliable and validated measure of changes in cognitive status [9]. Analysis of event-related-potentials (ERP) has been recommended for detecting early cognitive dysfunction, not usually detected by traditional methods of assessment of cognitive function [10]. Numerous clinical P300 studies [11,12,13], strongly suggest that this ERP component, elicited by auditory, visual, olfactory or somatosensory stimuli [14], may be clinically useful as an index of cognitive function. P300 wave may represent short duration memory functions and pre-attentive storage taking place in sensory cortex [15]. N200 wave may indicate an early cognitive elaboration concerning subject's attention orientation [16].

Particularly the latency of P300 and N200 components, have been found to increase with increasing cognitive deficit [10]. In the present study, these components of ERPs have been assessed along with MMSE for cognitive assessment in early and untreated cases of essential hypertension with increased pulse pressure, without overt dementia, to explore whether raised pulse pressure can be an independent risk factor for cognitive decline. It was anticipated that if such an association is found, it may prove to be a predictive marker, which will help plan timely intervention to preserve cognitive function with an individual’s increasing age.

Methods
A cross sectional, case control study was conducted wherein subjects were selected by screening patients from the outpatient departments of Lok Nayak Hospital by measurement of right arm blood pressure by a mercury sphygmomanometer. Sixty-five hypertensive males, aged 30-50 years with pulse pressure more than 60 mmHg were recruited into the study [17]. Prior to participation, all participants gave their written, informed consent for the study, which had received approval of the Institutional Ethics Committee. The importance of demographic variables has been stressed when examining association between BP and cognitive performance [18] and therefore, demographic data such as age (in years), education (in years) and occupation (level) was carefully recorded for each subject. Occupation was coded on a scale from 1 (unskilled) to 6 (executive/professional). Body mass index (BMI) was calculated for all subjects.

Subjects were restricted to age group 30-50 years as age related changes in event related potentials have been documented [19]. Females were excluded as ERPs are known to vary with the menstrual cycle [20]. Only those subjects not on any antihypertensive medication were included. Those with any obvious vascular or psychiatric disease, secondary hypertension or diabetes were excluded by careful recording of history, physical examination and by doing random blood sugar levels, serum electrolytes, serum creatinine, serum urea, lipid profile, urine examination 9for protein, glucose, blood and microscopic examination), chest X-ray, ECG and renal ultrasound. Overweight and obesity were classified according to 1998 National Institute of Health Guidelines [21].

Blood Pressure Measurement
Blood pressure was measured twice on the same patient, once on the screening day, and again on the test day. It was measured on the right arm in sitting position, as per the recommendations of the Canadian Hypertension Education Program [22]. Mean of the two readings was taken. Pulse pressure was defined as the difference between the systolic and the diastolic blood pressure. Only those subjects with pulse pressure more than 60 mmHg were included.

Sixty-five age and sex matched non-hypertensive controls, with pulse pressure less than 60 mmHg who were apparently healthy by history and thorough physical examination were also included in the study. MMSE and ERPs were assessed for this group also.

MMSE is a validated and reliable test for cognitive mental state and is useful in quantitatively estimating the severity of cognitive impairment and in serially documenting cognitive change [23]. The global cognitive assessment was based on the MMSE performed by the physician. A value less than 24 was considered to indicate cognitive impairment [23].

Event Related Potentials Recording
ERPs are generated by patient’s response to auditory, visual or other sensory stimuli. Auditory stimuli have been used in this study. An auditory cognitive ERP is generated by playing a baseline series of frequently occurring rhythmic auditory stimuli for a subject and then presenting secondary auditory stimuli (rare stimulus) at random. The subject mentally counts the secondary auditory “oddball” (rare) stimuli, and this specific intellectual function generates a discrete waveform of cognitive evoked response called the P300 component because its latency is about 300 ms (millisecond) after the stimulus [24].

EBNeuro machine (Evoked potential measuring system- Galileo NT) supplied by Firenze, Italy was used to record the Evoked Potentials. The evoked potentials were recorded as per the guidelines of International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiologists (IFCN) [25]. The examination was conducted in uniform conditions in all the subjects. The subjects assumed a comfortable position in a standard audiometric, sound proof, and air conditioned room so that they were relaxed. During the P300 recording session subject was instructed to fixate his eyes on a particular spot on the ceiling in order to avoid artifacts due to eye movements and improve his concentration and attention to target stimulus. Silver/silver chloride disc electrodes were used. First the scalp sites were cleaned with a cotton pad moistened with alcohol. The site was wiped with dry gauze to remove the oil from the skin. The site was rubbed with an appropriate amount of Neuprep skin prepping jelly to abrade the skin for impedance reduction and the site was further cleaned with a dry piece of gauze. The disc electrodes with 10-20 EEG conducting paste were applied to the sites and covered with a swab of cotton and the edges of the cotton pressed. The active electrodes were placed using the 10-20 international system on Fz, Pz and Cz sites referred to linked earlobes with a forehead ground. Electrode to skin impedance was kept below 5 kilo-ohms. The method was fully explained to all the subjects to get the best compliance.

P300 was measured in response to random application of two types of stimuli. “Odd-ball” acoustic paradigm was adopted, 200 stimuli were presented of which 40 were the rare or the target stimuli. 80 percent of the tones were frequent (500 Hz stimuli) and 20 percent were rare (2000 Hz stimuli), randomly received by patients. Acoustic stimuli were of 80 dB, each.

Linear tone with a starting condensation phase with a plateau phase of 100ms, rise/fall of 10ms and a rate of once every one second was used. The signals were in phase at the two ears. The Galileo NT settings were selected so as to filter the evoked responses to the frequent and the rare stimuli with a band pass of 0.1-20 Hz and averaged simultaneously for 40 responses. 

The P300 wave was identified as the largest positive peak occurring for all electrode sites after the N100-P200-N200 complex with latency more than 250ms. The latency and amplitude of the waveform were recorded. The latency was calculated from the point of stimulation.

The quality of cognitive response is related to amplitude and latency of the P300 wave, with higher amplitudes and shorter latencies being associated with improved cognitive function [26].

Statistical analysis
Data was analyzed using SPSS 17.0 software (SPSS, Chicago, Illinois, USA). Groupwise descriptive statistics of parameters are displayed as mean ± SD. The two groups were compared using unpaired student t test. P value <0.05 was considered significant. Pearson’s linear correlation coefficient was calculated by preparing a correlation matrix. Only those correlations with p<0.05 were considered significant.

Result
The characteristics of the study and control subjects have been summarized in Table 1. There was a significant difference between the systolic BP of cases and controls (p<0.001), and in the diastolic BP value (p<0.05). The pulse pressure was also significantly p<0.001) different between cases and controls. BMI, heart rate, education and occupation were similar in the two groups. Figure 1 shows the P300 and N200 waveforms generated during the study. The findings of the cognitive evaluation are summarized for both groups in Table 2. A significant positive correlation was derived by calculating Pearson’s linear correlation between N200 latency at Pz and pulse pressure in cases (r = 0.571, p<0.001). There was also a positive correlation between P300 latency at Pz and pulse pressure (r = 0.803, p<0.001). There was no correlation between the P300 amplitude and pulse pressure. MMSE was not different in the two groups.

Table 1- Characteristics of the study and control population

 

Controls (n=65)

Cases (n=65)

p value

Age (years)

39.32  ±  7.59

43.22 ± 6.32

NS

Systolic BP, mmHg

123.30 ± 12.48

159.84 ± 14.82

<0.001

Diastolic BP, mmHg

81.36 ± 9.12

84.07 ± 10.95

<0.05

Pulse pressure (mm Hg)

43.87 ± 8.79

70.76 ± 11.50

<0.001

BMI (kg/m­­­2)       

24.99 ± 3.99

28.47 ± 4.53

NS

Heart Rate (/min)

67.45 ± 11

70.75 ± 11.41

NS

Education (years)

10.5 ± 3.9

8.6 ± 4.6

NS

Occupation (Level 1-6)

3.5 ± 1.8

4.2 ± 2.8

NS

Table 2- Evoked potentials and MMSE in the two groups

 

 

Controls
(n=65)

Cases
(n=65)

P value

N200 latency (ms)

Fz

193.4 ± 11.78

243.21 ± 9.33

<0.01

 

Cz

187.3 ± 12.36

231.0 ± 12.57

<0.01

 

Pz

198.3 ± 15.21

248.22 ± 10.51

<0.001

P300 latency (ms)

Fz

310.2 ± 16.71

367.72 ± 14.35

<0.01

 

Cz

305.3 ± 16.10

361.36 ± 16.77

<0.01

 

Pz

316.1 ± 14.55

375.45 ± 14.98

<0.001

P300 amplitude (ms)

Fz

5.75 ± 1.32

6.71 ± 2.56

NS

 

Cz

6.12 ± 1.45

5.30 ± 2.11

<0.01

 

Pz

6.45 ± 1.88

5.67 ± 2.61

<0.01

MMSE score/30

 

29 ± 1

27 ± 3

NS


Figure 1- P300 and N200 waveforms recorded at three electrode sites, Fz, Cz and Pz.

Discussion
Our primary goal in this study was to assess the effect of increased pulse pressure in patients of untreated essential hypertension without dementia, on cognition using MMSE and evoked potentials. Our cases and controls had comparable age, BMI, heart rate, education and occupational levels and they were all males. The cases had wide pulse pressure as compared to the controls. Out of the 65 hypertensive cases, 16 had isolated systolic hypertension. The MMSE score was similar in the two groups and can be attributed to the fact that it has a limited ability to pick up sub-clinical cognitive impairment [9]. None of the cases had a score less than 24 as dementia was one of our exclusion criteria. Evoked potentials in cases showed an increase in N200 and P300 latency at all three electrode sites, Fz, Cz and Pz. Amplitude of P300 was also increased at Cz and Pz. There was a significant correlation between N200 latency and P300 latency at Pz and pulse pressure. Therefore, this study indicates a significant association between raised pulse pressure and cognitive impairment as indicated by prolonged latencies. In the absence of obvious clinical cognitive impairment as indicated by comparable MMSE scores, the presence of prolonged latencies on ERP testing indicates presence of subclinical cognitive impairment in untreated hypertensive population with wide pulse pressure.

Several mechanisms may explain the present finding of an association between raised pulse pressure and cognitive impairment. Pulse pressure and arterial stiffness have been related to atherosclerosis in large [27] and small vessels [28]. Vascular mechanisms of cognitive impairment involve small vessel diseases, which are associated with small infarcts (lacunae), white matter lesions, and cortical brain atrophy. Cognitive impairment may be the direct consequence of ischaemic brain lesions, depending on the volume, location, and number of these vascular lesions [29]. In addition, abnormalities of the white matter may be associated with cognitive impairment [30]. An increased pulse pressure has been associated with the prevalence and severity of cerebral white matter lesions and in a recent study, aortic stiffness appeared as an independent predictor of stroke in patients with essential hypertension [30]. Indeed the presence of arteriosclerosis or lipohyalinosis of small vessels might be the origin of vascular cerebral lesions or chronic hypoperfusion of the white matter and thus contribute to the development of cognitive impairment.

Age related confounding factors affecting blood pressure and cognition [31] have been excluded in the present study by restricting the age group from 30-50 years. To screen a population for hypertension and to find untreated cases is difficult, which has resulted in a restricted number of cases in this study.

This study has a few limitations. Information on vascular diseases was taken from the medical records of the patients. This might have led to the inclusion of some undiagnosed mild cases of vascular diseases. In addition, this is a cross sectional study, so does not have follow-up data on blood pressure and medication patterns or data on newly identified risk factors which may modify the effect of pulse pressure on cognition. Hence, despite the best of efforts to exclude several major confounders, some unknown confounding effects might still have been present.

This study indicates that increased pulse pressure, in patients of essential hypertension in the age group of 30-50 years, without overt dementia, is associated with prolonged latencies on ERPs , which can be taken to represent sub-clinical cognitive impairment. These results are similar to those of Papaliagkas et al [32] that P300 and N200 latencies can be used as measures of mild cognitive impairment independent of the MMSE score. These results emphasise the necessity of longitudinal studies to determine whether therapeutic options aimed at reducing pulse pressure can maintain optimal cognitive function or attenuate its decline with increasing age.

Key Points
  • The ill-effects of blood pressure on cardiovascular events and cognition is widely known
  • The present study has studied the effect of pulse pressure on cognition in untreated hypertensives
  • Wide pulse pressure is associated with sub-clinical cognitive dysfunction as evidenced in the form of prolonged latencies on event-related potentials

References

  1. The Joint National Committee on Prevention, Detection, Evaluation and Treatment of High Blood Pressure. The sixth report of the Joint National Committee on prevention, detection, evaluation, and treatment of high blood pressure (JNC VI). Arch Intern Med 1997;157:2413-46.
  2. Guidelines Subcommittee. World Health Organization-International Society of Hypertension guidelines for the management of hypertension. J Hypertens 1999;17:151-83.
  3. Nichols WW, O’Rourke MF. Properties of the arterial wall. In: McDonald’s Blood Flow in Arteries: Theoretical, Experimental and Clinical Principles. 4th edn London, England: Edward Arnold; 1998 pp. 125-9.
  4. Domanski MJ, Davis BR, Pfeffer MA, Kastantin M, Mitchell GF. Isolated systolic hypertension: prognostic information provided by pulse pressure. Hypertension 1999;34:375-80.
  5. Miller JA, Lever AF. Implications of pulse pressure as a predictor of cardiac risk in patients with hypertension. Hypertension 2000;36:907-11.
  6. Blacher J, Staessen JA, Girerd X, Gasowski J, Thijs L, Liu L, et al. Pulse pressure not mean pressure determines cardiovascular risk in older hypertensive patients. Arch Intern Med 2000;160:1085-9.
  7. Breteler MM. Vascular risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease: an epidemiologic perspective. Neurobiol Aging 2000;21:153-60.
  8. O’Brien JT, Erkinjuntti T, Reisberg B, Roman G, Sawada T, Pantoni L, et al. Vascular cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol 2003;2:89-98.
  9. Tombaugh TN, McDowell IM, Kristjansson B, Hubley AM. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and the modified MMSE (3MS): a psychometric comparison and normative data. Psychological Assessment 1996;8:48-59.
  10. Buziashvili YI, Aleksakhina YA, Ambat’ello SG, Matskeplishvili ST. Use of P300 cognitive evoked potentials in the diagnosis of impairments of higher mental functions after cardiac surgery in conditions of cardiopulmonary bypass. Neurosci Behav Physiol 2006:36:115-8.
  11. Yamaguchi S, Knight RT. P300 generation by novel somatosensory stimuli. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1991;78:50-5.
  12. Bennys K, Portet F, Touchon J, Rondouin G. Diagnostic value of event-related evoked potentials N200 and P300 subcomponents in early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. J Clin Neurophysiol 2007;24:405-12.
  13. Golob EJ, Irimajiri R, Starr A. Auditory cortical activity in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: relationship to subtype and conversion to dementia. Brain 2007;130:740-52.
  14. Polich J. P300 in clinical applications. In: Electroencephalography: basic principles, clinical applications and related fields. Niedermayer E, Lopes de la Silva, Urban F (eds). Schwartzenberger, Baltimore-Munich;1999:1073-91.
  15. Banquet JP, Guenther W, Smith M. Probability processing in depressed patients. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl 1987;40:645-50.
  16. Piras MR, Magnano I, Canu ED, Paulus KS, Satta WM, Soddu A, et al. Longitudinal study of cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: neuropsychological, neuroradiological, and neurophysiological findings. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003;74:878-85.
  17. Madhavan S, Ooi WL, Cohen H, Alderman MH. Relation of pulse pressure and blood pressure reduction to the incidence of myocardial infarction. Hypertension 1994;23:395-401.
  18. Elias MF, Robbins MA, Budge MM, Elias PK, Hermann BA, Dore GA. Studies of aging, hypertension and cognitive functioning, with contributions from the Maine-Syracuse study. In: Costa PT, Seigler IC, editors. Advances in cell aging and gerontology, Vol 15, Recent advances in psychology and aging. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2004.p.89-131.
  19. Frodl T, Juckel G, Gallinat J, Bottlender R, Riedel M, Preuss U, et al. Dipole localization of P300 and normal aging. Brain Topogr 2000:13:3-9.
  20. O'Reilly MA, Cunningham CJ, Lawlor BA, Walsh CD, Rowan MJ. The effect of the menstrual cycle on electrophysiological and behavioral measures of memory and mood. Psychophysiology 2004;41:592-603.
  21. Clinical guidelines on the identification, evaluation, and treatment of overweight and obesity in adults-the evidence report. National Institute of Health. Obes Res 1998; 6 (Suppl 2); 51S-209S.
  22. Hemmelgarn BR, McAlister FA, Grover S, Myers MG, McKay DW, Bolli P, et al. The 2006 Canadian Hypertension Education Program recommendations for the management of hypertension: Part I – Blood pressure measurement, diagnosis and assessment of risk. Can J Cardiol 2006;22:573–81.
  23. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-98.
  24. Qureshi N, Babbar R. Effect of ramipril therapy on cognitive functions in cases of mild to moderate essential hypertension. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 2007;51:40-8.
  25. Nuwer MR, Lehmann D, Silva FLD, Matsuoka S, Sutherling W. IFCN guidelines for topographic and frequency analysis of EEGs and EPs. Report of an IFCN committee. International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994;91:1-5.
  26. Stivelman JC. Benefits of anaemia treatment on cognitive function. Nephrol Dial Transplant 2000;15(suppl 3): 29-35
  27. Boutouyrie P, Bussy C, Lacolley P, Girerd X, Laloux B, Laurent S. Association between local pulse pressure, mean blood pressure, and large artery remodeling. Circulation 1999;100:1387-93.
  28. Baumbach GL, Siems JE, Heistad DD. Effects of local reduction in pressure on distensibility and composition of cerebral arterioles. Circ Res 1991;68:338-51.
  29. Pasquier F, Leys D. Why are stroke patients prone to develop dementia? J Neurol 1997;244:135-42.
  30. Breteler MM, van Swieten JC, Bots ML, Grobbee DE, Claus JJ, van den Hout JH. Cerebral white matter lesions, vascular risk factors, and cognitive function in a population-based study: the Rotterdam study. Neurology 1994;44:1246-52.
  31. Franklin SS, Gustin W 4th, Wong ND, Larson MG, Weber MA, Kannel WB, et al. Hemodynamic patterns of age-related changes in blood pressure. The Framingham Heart Study. Circulation 1997;96:308-15.
  32. Papaliagkas V, Kimiskidis V, Tsolaki M, Anogianakis G. Usefulness of event-related potentials in the assessment of mild cognitive impairment. BMC Neurosci 2008;9:107.

Conflict of interest: None declared.


 
 
Copyright © Indian Journal of Medical Specialities Trust