O P Tandon*, Varun Malhotra**, Shruti Tandon†
*Department of Physiology, University College of Medical Sciences, Delhi, **Department of Physiology, Vinayaka Mission Medical College, Salem, †Department of Periodontics, Maulana Azad Institute of Dental Sciences, New Delhi
Corresponding Author: Dr O P Tandon 70-C Masjid Moth II New Delhi 110048.
E mail: droptandon@gmail.com
Abstract
The classical methods of forensic science like finger printing, polygraphic lie detector test, narcoanalysis and
DNA finger printing are being questioned for their reliability and accuracy. Newer developments in
investigating crime are evolving like the use of event related evoked potentials to retrieve the sequence of
events of crime as recorded in the brain of the criminal/accused.
The principle of event related evoked potentials is simple. In an oddball paradigm two stimuli of varying
characteristics are presented to the individual, one repeated frequently and the other being rare, is given
randomly at infrequent intervals. The subject is asked to respond to the infrequent or target stimulus by
pressing a button. The response to target stimulus is recorded as P3 wave of the cognitive evoked potentials.
P3 wave of the ERPs has wide applications in cognitive forensic science and interrogative polygraphy in
particular. Late vertex positivity in ERP reflects guilt detection in a test known as guilty knowledge. Memory
and encoding related multifaceted electroencephalographic responses (MERMER) have been used to
determine whether the subject had relevant information (committing of crime) in his brain, after presenting
him with probing (relevant), target and non-target stimuli. Two high amplitude P3 waves are recorded in
response to target and probing stimuli in case of a knowledgeable person and only one P3 in case of an
innocent person.
The newly emerging, late responses to ERP is a step forward to quantify the thought processing feature of the
brain. Brain wave science has emerged as a new discipline with promising application in field of crime
investigation.
Key words: Event Related Evoked Potentials, MERMER, Target Stimulus, Probing Stimulus. P3 Wave, Long
Latency Responses, Lie detection
|
Forensic science employs classical methods like
finger printing, polygraphic lie detector test,
narcoanalysis and DNA finger printing but questions
are being posed for their reliability and accuracy.
There have been many recent advances in the
science of investigating crime. One of these is the
use of event related evoked potentials to retrieve
the sequence of events of crime as recorded in the
brain of the criminal or accused. Event related
evoked potentials are long latency endogenous
cerebral potentials indicating higher brain functions
emanating from association and limbic areas. A brief
overview of the classical methods is given below-
Polygraph lie detector: Sensors are attached to the body, and the polygraph machine records changes in
breathing, blood pressure, pulse and perspiration.
Control questions are asked first, that sets up a
pattern of how an individual responds when giving
true and false answers. A lie should cause a certain
amount of stress that produces changes in several
involuntary physiological reactions (such as blood
pressure rising) [1]. However suspects dupe it by
taking drugs like smoking and nicotine that calm the
nerves and autonomic responses.
A DNA test determines only whether two DNA
samples match, it does not determine whether the
investigator did an effective job of collecting DNA
from the crime scene. Also, DNA material may not be
enough at the scene of crime also.
Human brain is like a videocamera. It absorbs like a
sponge from the environment. It is constantly being
bombarded with mostly visual, auditory and some
somatosensory stimuli from the ambient
environment. These stimuli from the outside world
result in an electrical evoked responses and events
which are stored in the subconscious mind. These
can be retrieved by brain fingerprinting. In the
latter context, the controlled administration of
intravenous hypnotic medications is called"narcosynthesis" or "narcoanalysis". It may be
used to procure diagnostically- or therapeuticallyvital
information, and to provide patients with a
functional respite from catatonia. The application
of narcoanalysis has very limited use today as it
requires medication and subjects cooperation.
Event related evoked potentials (ERP)
ERP technique is promising as it can provide an
extraordinary means to find evidence which is not
seen with the naked eye. This testing when used in
reading criminal/accused’s brain is known as brain
fingerprinting and these ERP waves can see the truth
locked in the accused’s memory.
“Brain fingerprinting" is a computer-based test
based on a very simple principle. The memory areas
of human brain respond to the sight of familiar
stimuli with a distinct change in electrical activity.
It is designed to discover the P300 as a response to
stimuli related to the crime or other investigated
situation. It is used to document, and provide
evidence of guilty knowledge regarding crimes, and
to identify individuals with a specific training or
expertise such as members of dormant terrorist
cells or bomb makers . Thus it is an
electrophysiological method of detemining whether
certain information is stored in a brain or not stored.
If the person has committed a crime he will have
certain information relevant to the crime stored in
his brain which can be retrieved with brain
fingerprinting. The ERPs have also been used to
evaluate higher brain functions in endocrine
disorders [2], anaemia [3], cognitive information
[4], epilepsy [5], chronic renal disease [6] and also
as a means of early detection of cognitive
impairment in caffeine users [7]. The technique
uses the well known fact that an electrical signal
known as P300 is emitted from an individual's brain
beginning approximately 300 milliseconds after it is
confronted with a stimulus of special significance,
e.g. a rare vs a common stimulus the frequency of which the subject is asked to count. The person to
be tested wears a special headband with electronic
sensors that measures the EEG from several
locations on the scalp. The subject views stimuli
consisting of words, phrases, or pictures presented
on a computer screen [8]. If he is attentive to the
rare stimulus A positive P wave at 300 ms latency is
recorded (P300).
MERMER
While researching the P300, Dr. Farwell created a
more detailed test that not only includes the P300,
but also observes the stimulus response up to 1400
ms after the stimulus. He calls this brain response a
MERMER, memory and encoding related
multifaceted electroencephalographic response.
The P300, an electrically positive component, is
maximal at the midline parietal area of the head and
has a peak latency of approximately 300 ms. The
MERMER includes the P300 and also an electrically
negative component, with an onset latency of
approximately 800-1200ms [8,9]. According to Dr.
Farwell, the MERMER includes additional features
involving changes in the frequency of the EEG signal,
but for the purposes of signal detection and
practical application, the MERMER is sufficiently
characterized by the P300 and the following
negative component in the brain response. Stimuli
are of three types: 1) “irrelevant” stimuli that are
irrelevant to the investigated situation and to the
test subject, 2) “target” stimuli that are relevant to
the investigated situation and are known to the
subject, and 3) “probe” stimuli that are relevant to
the investigated situation and that the subject
denies knowing. Probes contain information that is
known only to the perpetrator and investigators,
and not to the general public or to an innocent
suspect who was not at the scene of the crime.
Before the test, the scientist identifies the targets
to the subject, and makes sure that he/she knows
these relevant stimuli. The scientist also makes sure
that the subject does not know the probes for any
reason unrelated to the crime, and that the subject
denies knowing the probes. The subject is told why
the probes are significant (e.g., “You will see
several items, one of which is the murder weapon”),
but is not told which items are the probes and which
are irrelevant [9,10]. By comparing the responses to
the different types of stimuli, the brain
fingerprinting system mathematically computes a
determination of “information present” (the subject knows the crime-relevant information
contained in the probe stimuli) or “information
absent” (the subject does not know the
information) and a statistical confidence for the
determination. This determination is
mathematically computed, and does not involve the
subjective judgment of the scientist. MERMERs are
thus elicited by probe stimuli only in the subjects
who have participated in the investigated event, by
target stimuli and not by irrelevant stimuli. In short
it means, a person who has committed the crime or
the one who is knowledgeable about the true
events, MERMER is seen which consists of two welldefined
P300 responses, one to relevant stimulus
and the other to probing stimulus. Information
absent response does not show MERMER but only one
distinct P300 response to relevant (rare) stimulus.
The probing stimulus is irrelevant for him as he has
no knowledge of the crime.
Brain fingerprinting vs polygraphic test
The application of this in brain fingerprinting is to
detect the P300 as a response to stimuli related to
the crime or other investigated situation, e.g. a
murder weapon, victim's face, or knowledge of the
internal workings of a terrorist cell [8,11,12]. Since
brain fingerprinting uses cognitive brain responses,
it does not depend on the emotions of the subject,
nor is it affected by emotional responses [13,14,15].
Brain fingerprinting is fundamentally different from
the polygraph (lie-detector), which measures
emotion-based physiological signals such as heart
rate, sweating, and blood pressure [8]. Also, unlike
polygraph testing, it does not attempt to determine
whether or not the subject is lying or telling the
truth. Rather, it measures the subject’s brain
response to relevant words, phrases, or pictures to
detect whether or not the relevant information is
stored in the subject’s brain [10,12,16].
Application
Brain Fingerprinting Testing has been ruled
admissible in court [17]. The Brain Fingerprinting
system tests for knowledge of salient features of a
crime stored in the brain. Scientists know that we
don't remember everything, but we do remember
significant features of major events, like
committing a serious crime. By scientifically
determining what is stored in a suspect's brain, Brain
Fingerprinting testing provides evidence that can be used by judges and juries in making a determination
as to whether the suspect committed the crime or
not.
Brain Fingerprinting testing is used by FBI agents to
apply this science in bringing criminals to justice
and freeing innocent suspects [13,14,18]. It has
been applied not only in rigorous laboratory studies
but also in over 100 real-life cases, for a US
intelligence agency and for the US Navy.
As with other scientific evidence, Brain
Fingerprinting testing does not prove guilt or
innocence per se. It provides information about
what is stored in the suspect's brain. A judge or jury
can utilize this information in making the legal
determination of guilt or innocence
Limitations
Brain fingerprinting detects information-processing
brain responses that reveal what information is
stored in the subject’s brain. It does not detect how
that information got there. This fact has
implications for how and when the technique can be
applied. In a case where a suspect claims not to have
been at the crime scene and has no legitimate
reason for knowing the details of the crime, and
investigators have information that has not been
released to the public, brain fingerprinting can
determine objectively whether or not the subject
possesses that information. In such a case, brain
fingerprinting could provide useful evidence [12].
If, however, the suspect knows everything that the
investigators know about the crime for some
legitimate reason, then the test cannot be applied.
There are several circumstances in which this may
be the case. If a suspect acknowledges being at the
scene of the crime, but claims to be a witness and
not a perpetrator, then the fact that he knows
details about the crime would not be incriminating.
There would be no reason to conduct a test, because
the resulting “information present” response would
simply show that the suspect knew the details about
the crime – knowledge which he already admits and
which he gained at the crime scene whether he was
a witness or a perpetrator.
Another case where brain fingerprinting is not
applicable would be one wherein a suspect and an
alleged victim – say, of an alleged sexual assault –
agree on the details of what was said and done, but disagree on the intent of the parties. Brain
fingerprinting detects only information, and not
intent. The fact that the suspect knows the
uncontested facts of the circumstance does not tell
us which party’s version of the intent is correct.
In a case where the suspect knows everything that
the investigators know because he has been exposed
to all available information in a previous trial, there
is no available information with which to construct
probe stimuli, so a test cannot be conducted. Even
in a case where the suspect knows many of the
details about the crime, however, it is sometimes
possible to discover salient information that the
perpetrator must have encountered in the course of
committing the crime, but the suspect claims not to
know and would not know if he were innocent. This
was the case with Terry Harrington [10]. By
examining reports, interviewing witnesses, and
visiting the crime scene and surrounding areas, Dr.
Farwell was able to discover salient features of the
crime that Harrington had never been exposed to at
his previous trials. The brain fingerprinting test
showed that the record in Harrington’s brain did not
contain these salient features of the crime, but only
the details about the crime that he had learned
after the fact.
Obviously, in structuring a brain fingerprinting test,
a scientist must avoid including information that has
been made public. Detecting that a suspect knows
information he obtained by reading a newspaper
would not be of use in a criminal investigation, and
standard brain fingerprinting procedures eliminate
all such information from the structuring of a test
[1,19]. News accounts containing many of the
details of a crime do not interfere with the
development of a brain fingerprinting test,
however; they simply limit the material that can be
tested. Even in highly publicized cases, there are
almost always many details that are known to the
investigators but not released to the public and
these can be used as stimuli to test the subject for
knowledge that he would have no way to know
except by committing the crime.
Another situation where brain fingerprinting is not
applicable is one where the authorities have no
information about what crime may have taken
place. For example, an individual may disappear
under circumstances where a specific suspect had a
strong motive to murder the individual. Without any
evidence, authorities do not know whether a murder took place, or the individual decided to take
a trip and tell no one, or some other criminal or noncriminal
event happened. If there is no known
information on which a suspect could be tested, a
brain fingerprinting test cannot be structured.
Similarly, brain fingerprinting is not applicable for
general screening, for example, in general preemployment
or employee screening wherein any
number of undesirable activities or intentions may
be relevant. If the investigators have no idea what
crime or undesirable act the individual may have
committed, there is no way to structure appropriate
stimuli to detect the telltale knowledge that would
result from committing the crime. Brain
fingerprinting can, however, be used for specific
screening or focused screening, when investigators
have some idea what they are looking for. For
example, brain fingerprinting can be used to detect
whether a person has knowledge that would identify
him as an FBI agent, an Al-Qaeda-trained terrorist, a
member of a criminal organization or terrorist cell,
or a bomb maker [12].
Brain fingerprinting simply detects information. No
questions are asked or answered during a brain
fingerprinting test. The subject neither lies nor tells
the truth during a brain fingerprinting test, and the
outcome of the test is unaffected by whether he has
lied or told the truth at any other time. The outcome
of “information present” or “information absent”
depends on whether the relevant information is
stored in the brain, and not on what the subject says
about it [19].
Key Points
Brain finger printing is a new technique which is
useful in various fields. Brain fingerprinting" is a
computer-based test that is designed to discover,
document, and provide evidence of guilty
knowledge regarding crimes, and to identify
individuals with a specific training or expertise
such as members of dormant terrorist cells or
bomb makers.
|
References
- Farwell LA, Donchin E. “The brain detector: P300 in the detection of deception.” sychophysiology 1986,24:434
- Anjana Y, Tandon OP, Vaney N, Madhu SV. Cognitive status in hypothyroid female patients: event related evoked potential study. Neuroendocrinol 2008;88:59-66.
- Bandhu R, Shankar N, Tandon OP, Madan N. Effect of iron therapy on cognition in anaemic school going children Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 2003;47:301-10.
- Tandon OP. Brain waves from diagnosis to crime detection. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 2003; 47(5 Suppl):10-1.
- Tandon OP, Duhan P. Event related evoked potentials in epileptic patients. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 2000;44:461-6.
- Dixit A, Vaney N, Tandon OP. Evaluation of cognitive brain function in caffeine users: P3 event related evoked potential study. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 2006;50:175-80.
- Madan P, Agarwal S, Kalra OP, Tandon OP. Effect of haemodialysis on cognitive functions in ESRD patients. Ren Fail 2007;29:699-703.
- Allen JJB, Iacono WG. A comparison of methods for the analysis of event-related potentials in deception detection. Psychophysiology 1997;34:234-40.
- CBS 60 Minutes: Mike Wallace interviews Dr. Lawrence Farwell, December 10, 2000.
- Druckman D, Lacey JI. Brain and cognition: some new technologies. Washington, D.C.:National Academy Press 1989.
- Farwell LA. Two new twists on the truth detector: brain-wave detection of occupational information. Psychophysiology 1992b; 29(4A):S3.
- Dalbey B. Brain Fingerprinting Testing Traps Serial Killer in Missouri. The Fairfield Ledger. Fairfield, IA, August, 1999, p 1.
- Abdollah T. Brain Fingerprinting– Picture-perfect crimes. Berkeley Medical Journal Issues, Spring 2003. Accessed July 20, 2008.
- Farwell LA. Method and Apparatus for Multifaceted Electroencephalographic Response Analysis (MERA). U.S. Patent #5,363,858, Nov. 15, 1994
- Farwell LA. Method and Apparatus for Truth Detection. U.S. Patent #5,406,956, April 18, 1995.
- Dale SS. THE BRAIN SCIENTIST: Climbing Inside the Criminal Mind. TIME Magazine, Nov. 26, 2001, pp 80-81.
- ABC-TV Good Morning America: Charles Gibson interviews Dr. Lawrence Farwell “Mind-Reading Technology Tests Subject's Guilt- Brain-Reading Technology Becomes New Tool in Courts,” March 9, 2004
- Farwell LA. Brain MERMERs: detection of FBI Agents and crime-relevant information with the Farwell MERA system. Proceedings of the International Security Systems Symposium,1993, Washington, D.C
- Farwell LA. Method for Electroencephalographic Information Detection. U.S. Patent #5,467,777, Nov. 21, 1995.
|